یک تحلیل طولی درباره ارتباط بین تواناییهای میان فرهنگی و سبکهای هویت فرهنگی
ترجمه نشده

یک تحلیل طولی درباره ارتباط بین تواناییهای میان فرهنگی و سبکهای هویت فرهنگی

عنوان فارسی مقاله: رابطه بین تواناییهای میان فرهنگی و سبکهای هویت فرهنگی: یک تحلیل متقاطع طولی
عنوان انگلیسی مقاله: The relationship between intercultural abilities and cultural identity styles: A longitudinal cross‐lagged analysis
مجله/کنفرانس: مجله بین المللی روانشناسی - International Journal Of Psychology
رشته های تحصیلی مرتبط: روانشناسی، علوم اجتماعی
گرایش های تحصیلی مرتبط: روانشناسی صنعتی و سازمانی، روانشناسی عمومی
کلمات کلیدی فارسی: سبک هویت متناوب، دو فرهنگی، سبک هویت ترکیبی، توانایی های بین فرهنگی، اثربخشی بین فرهنگی، طولی
کلمات کلیدی انگلیسی: Alternating identity style، Bicultural، Hybrid identity style، Intercultural abilities، Intercultural effectiveness، Longitudinal
شناسه دیجیتال (DOI): https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12592
دانشگاه: Centre for Applied Cross-Cultural Research, School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand
صفحات مقاله انگلیسی: 7
ناشر: وایلی - Wiley
نوع ارائه مقاله: ژورنال
نوع مقاله: ISI
سال انتشار مقاله: 2019
ایمپکت فاکتور: 1/538 در سال 2018
شاخص H_index: 53 در سال 2019
شاخص SJR: 0/740 در سال 2018
شناسه ISSN: 1464-066X
شاخص Quartile (چارک): Q2 در سال 2018
فرمت مقاله انگلیسی: PDF
وضعیت ترجمه: ترجمه نشده است
قیمت مقاله انگلیسی: رایگان
آیا این مقاله بیس است: خیر
آیا این مقاله مدل مفهومی دارد: ندارد
آیا این مقاله پرسشنامه دارد: ندارد
آیا این مقاله متغیر دارد: دارد
کد محصول: E13130
رفرنس: دارای رفرنس در داخل متن و انتهای مقاله
فهرست مطالب (انگلیسی)

Intercultural abilities and identity development

METHOD

Procedure and sample

Measures

Data analysis

RESULTS

Bivariate correlations

Cross-lagged analysis

DISCUSSION

REFERENCES

بخشی از مقاله (انگلیسی)

Intercultural abilities and identity development Chen and Starosta (1996) describe the ability to engage in culturally functional and appropriate behaviours as cultural adroitness or intercultural effectiveness. A wide range of abilities that support intercultural effectiveness have been identified, including the abilities to negotiate and protect the cultural identities of self and others, maintain flexibility, communicate competently across cultures, manage intercultural interactions and cultivate intercultural relationships (Chen, 2007; Imahori & Cupach, 2005; Portalla & Chen, 2010; Spitzberg, 2000). Beyond intercultural effectiveness, these abilities have also been discussed in terms of related and overlapping constructs such as intercultural competence (e.g. Leung, Ang, & Tan, 2014), cultural intelligence (e.g. Ang et al., 2007) and sociocultural adaptation (e.g. Wilson, Ward, Fetvadjiev, & Bethel, 2017). Despite the multiple approaches to defining and assessing intercultural abilities, research findings converge to show that those with greater abilities have better social relationships (e.g. quantity and quality of social contact; Ward & Kennedy, 1993; Zlobina, Basabe, Paez, & Furnham, 2006), report higher levels of psychological wellbeing (Ang et al., 2007; van Oudenhoven, Mol, & Van der Zee, 2003), are more successful in their jobs and find it easier to adjust to new work environments (Lee & Sukoco, 2010; van Oudenhoven et al., 2003). It is, however, far less understood what role culture-specific abilities play in integrating multicultural identities. For example, Lee’s (2010) study with expatriates examined the relationship between intercultural abilities and identification with the heritage and host cultures. Those reporting strong identification with both cultures had higher levels of intercultural effectiveness (measured as cultural appropriateness and communication effectiveness) than their peers who identified with only one or neither culture. Similarly, Thomas, Brannen, and Garcia (2010) found that bicultural university students scored significantly higher on cultural metacognition (i.e. awareness of and reflective thinking about cultural knowledge and interactions) than monoculturals. Benet-Martínez and Haritatos’s (2005) study with Chinese Americans also concluded that “individuals who report having overlapping or hyphenated cultural identities are more likely to participate in both cultures effectively” (p. 1033). While there is some empirical evidence linking intercultural abilities to bicultural engagement and identity integration, the direction of the relationship is unclear. Bicultural individuals might develop better intercultural abilities and display greater cultural adroitness because of their familiarity with and knowledge of multiple cultures (Lee, 2010; Thomas et al., 2010). This would suggest that both the hybrid and alternating identity styles are predictive of greater intercultural effectiveness over time, as they represent efforts to integrate two or more cultural identities by either combining cultural elements in a unique way or by emphasising them depending on situational demands. Although research to date has linked the alternating style to negative psychological outcomes, alternation and situated identities have been theorised to have potential benefits by enabling individuals to behave in accordance with cultural norms (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993; Noels & Clément, 2015), and, therefore, could be predictive of greater intercultural effectiveness.